Influencia del contexto en el desarrollo cognitivo infantil: revisión sistemática

Contenido principal del artículo

María A. Benítez, Lic.
Verónika Díaz Abraham, Ph. D.
Nadia R. Justel, Ph. D.

Resumen

(analítico)
Durante los últimos años diversas investigaciones analizaron la asociación entre contexto y desarrollo infantil. Un problema central es si el ambiente influye en el desarrollo cognitivo. El contexto está compuesto por diferentes niveles que interactúan entre sí e influyen en las personas: el microsistema, mesosistema, exosistema y el macrosistema. El objetivo de esta revisión sistemática es indagar acerca de los factores que pertenecen al contexto y modularían el desarrollo infantil. A través de una búsqueda de artículos empíricos en revistas científicas indexadas, se seleccionaron 22 artículos, los cuales fueron analizados mediante la comparación de las variables: población, país, instrumento de evaluación, constructo evaluado, frecuencia de evaluación y resultados. Los resultados muestran que los factores ambientales pertenecientes a cada nivel analizado tienen la capacidad de modular el desarrollo infantil.


Palabras clave:  Contexto cultural; desarrollo infantil; cognición.

Descargas

Los datos de descargas todavía no están disponibles.

Detalles del artículo

Cómo citar
Benítez, M. A., Diaz-Abrahan, V., & Justel, N. R. (2023). Influencia del contexto en el desarrollo cognitivo infantil: revisión sistemática. Revista Latinoamericana De Ciencias Sociales, Niñez Y Juventud, 21(2), 1–27. https://doi.org/10.11600/rlcsnj.21.2.5321
Sección
Segunda Parte Volumen 21 No. 2
Biografía del autor/a

María A. Benítez, Lic., Universidad de Palermo, Argentina

Laboratorio Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Centro de Investigación en Neurociencia y Neuropsicología, Universidad de Palermo, Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas, Buenos Aires, Argentina. Lic. en Musicoterapia, Universidad de Buenos Aires. 0000-0001-5231-8430. H5: 2. Correo electrónico: mariabenitez@conicet.gov.ar

Verónika Díaz Abraham, Ph. D., Universidad de Palermo, Argentina

Laboratorio Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Centro de Investigación en Neurociencia y
Neuropsicología, Universidad de Palermo, Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas, Buenos Aires, Argentina. Dra. en Neurociencias, Universidad Nacional de Córdoba. 0000-0001-5003-4274. H5: 7. Correo electrónico: abrahanveronika@conicet.gov.ar

Nadia R. Justel, Ph. D., Universidad de Palermo, Argentina

Laboratorio Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Centro de Investigación en Neurociencia y
Neuropsicología, Universidad de Palermo, Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas, Buenos Aires, Argentina. Dra. en Psicología, Universidad Nacional de Córdoba. 0000-0002-0145-3357. H5: 16. Correo electrónico: nadiajustel@conicet.gov.ar

Citas

*Amir, D., & McAuliffe, K. (2020). Cross-cultural, developmental psychology: Integrating approaches and key insights. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(5), 430-444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.06.006

*Angoff, W. H. (1988). The nature-nurture debate, aptitudes, and group differences. American Psychologist, 43(9), 713-720. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.43.9.713

*Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Matute, E., & Guajardo, S. (2005). The influence of the parents’ educational level on the development of executive functions. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(1), 539-560. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2801_5

*Armstrong-Carter, E., Trejo, S., Hill, L. J., Crossley, K. L., Mason, D., & Domingue, B. W. (2020). The earliest origins of genetic nurture: The prenatal environment mediates the association between maternal genetics and child development. Psychological Science, 31(7), 781-791. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620917209

Atkinson, A. B., & Marlier, E. (2010). Analysing and measuring social inclusion in a global context. United Nations.

*Barrett, H. (2020). Towards a cognitive science of the human: Cross-cultural approaches and their urgency. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(8), 620-638. https://doi.org/gg3dv6

*Bartkowski, J. P., Xu, X., & Levin, M. (2008). Religion and child development: Evidence from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study. Social Science Research, 37(1), 18-36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2007.02.001

*Bender, A., & Beller, S. (2019). The cultural fabric of human causal cognition. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(6), 922-940. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619863055

*Blair, C., & Raver, C. (2012). Child development in the context of adversity: Experiential canalization of brain and behavior. American Psychologist, 67(4), 309-318. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027493

*Blanco, V., Pérez, C. A., & Cova, F. M. (2020). Parental well-being of mothers and fathers in Latin America: A systematic review of the literature. Child Care in Practice, 28(3), 411-437. https://doi.org/10.1080/13575279.2020.1723064

*Brody, G. H., Dorsey, S., Forehand, R., & Armistead, L. (2002). Unique and protective contributions of parenting and classroom processes to the adjustment of African American children living in single-parent families. Child Development, 73(1), 274-286. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00405

*Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. (1994). Nature-nuture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological Review, 101(4), 568-586. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.101.4.568

*Bronfenbrenner, U., & Evans, G. W. (2000). Developmental science in the 21st century: Emerging questions, theoretical models, research designs and empirical findings. Social Development, 9(1), 115-125. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00114

*Callaghan, T., Rochat, P., Lillard, A., Claux, M. L., Odden, H., Itakura, S., Tapanya, S., & Singh, S. (2005). Synchrony in the onset of mental-state reasoning: Evidence from five cultures. Psychological Science, 16(5), 378-384. https://doi.org/fhvf5q

*Caplan, B., Morgan, J., Noroña, A., Tung, I., Lee, S. S., & Baker, B. L. (2019). The nature and nurture of social development: The role of 5-HTTLPR and gene-parenting interactions. Journal of Family Psychology, 33(8), 927-937. https://doi.org/j6qj

*Catale, C., Willems, S., Lejeune, C., & Meulemans, T. (2012). Parental educational level influence on memory and executive performance in children. European Review of Applied Psychology, 62(3), 161-171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2012.04.003

*Chakraborty, L., Rus, H., Henstra, D., Thistlethwaite, J., & Scott, D. (2020). A place-based socioeconomic status index: Measuring social vulnerability to flood hazards in the context of environmental justice. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction, 43, 101394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdrr.2019.101394

*Conway, A., Waldfogel, J., & Wang, Y. (2018). Parent education and income gradients in children’s executive functions at kindergarten entry. Children and Youth Services Review, 91, 329-337. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.06.009

*Cozzi, P., Putnam, S., Menesini, E., Gartstein, M., Aureli, T., Calussi, P., & Montirosso, R. (2013). Studying cross-cultural differences in temperament in toddlerhood: United States of America (US) and Italy. Infant Behavior & Development, 36(3), 480-483. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.03.014

*de Castro, F., Vázquez-Salas, R., Villalobos, A., Rubio-Codina, M., Prado, E., Sánchez Ferrer, J., Romero, M., & Shamah-Levy, T. (2019). Contexto y resultados del desarrollo infantil temprano en niños y niñas de 12 a 59 meses en México. Salud Pública de México, 61(6), 775-786. https://doi.org/10.21149/10560

*Early, D. M., Bryant, D. M., Pianta, R. C., Clifford, R. M., Burchinal, M. R., Ritchie, S., Howes, C., & Barbarin, O. (2006). Are teachers’ education, major, and credentials related to classroom quality and children’s academic gains in pre-kindergarten? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21(2), 174-195. https://doi.org/cvx24s

*Fernald, L. C., & Hidrobo, M. (2011). Effect of Ecuador’s cash transfer program (Bono de Desarrollo Humano) on child development in infants and toddlers: A randomized effectiveness trial. Social Science & Medicine, 72(9), 1437-1446. https://doi.org/fbg9b4

*Fernández-Rodríguez, M. (2021). Sustentos teóricos que fundamentan la influencia del contexto comunitario en el desarrollo infantil integral de los niños de 0-5 años. Innova Research Journal, 6(3.1), 114-130. https://doi.org/j6qr

*Fröjd, S., Kaltiala-Heino, R., & Rimpelä, M. (2007). The association of parental monitoring and family structure with diverse maladjustment outcomes in middle adolescent boys and girls. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 61(4), 296-303. https://doi.org/cxzntk

*Green, C., Strobach, T., & Schubert, T. (2014). On methodological standards in training and transfer experiments. Psychological Research, 78, 756-772. https://doi.org/j6qs

*Hackman, D. A., Betancourt, L. M., Gallop, R., Romer, D., Brodsky, N. L., Hurt, H., & Farah, M. J. (2014). Mapping the trajectory of socioeconomic disparity in working memory: Parental and neighborhood factors. Child Development, 85(4), 1433-1445. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12242

*Han, S., & Northoff, G. (2008). Culture-sensitive neural substrates of human cognition: A transcultural neuroimaging approach. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 646-654. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2456

*Han, W.-J., & Fox, L. (2011). Parental work schedules and children’s cognitive trajectories. Journal of Marriage and Family, 73(5), 962-980. https://doi.org/fvtzbm

*Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H. R., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2008). Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychological Science, 19(1), 12- 17. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02038.x

*Horn, J. M., Loehlin, J. C., & Willerman, L. (1976). Nature-nurture and intelligence: The twin and adoption studies agree. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae: Twin Research, 25(1), 195-197. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0001566000014124

*Johnson, S. B., Riis, J. L., & Noble, K. G. (2016). State of the art review: Poverty and the developing brain. Pediatrics, 137(4), e20153075. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-3075

*Joshi, P., & Bogen, K. (2007). Nonstandard schedules and young children’s behavioral outcomes among working low-income families. Journal of Marriage and Family, 69(1), 139-156. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2006.00350.x

*Kullmann, L. (2020). Systematic reviews: incentives, pressures, and possible limitations. International Journal of Rehabilitation Research, 43(2), 99-101. https://doi.org/j6qz

*Kuwabara, M., & Smith, L. B. (2012). Cross-cultural differences in cognitive development: Attention to relations and objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(1), 20- 35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.04.009

*Lau, F. (2017). Methods for correlational studies. En F. Lau, & C. Kuziemsky (Ed.), Hand-book of eHealth evaluation: An evidence-based approach (pp. 213-226). University of Victoria. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK481614/

*Lawson, G. M., & Farah, M. J. (2017). Executive function as a mediator between SES and academic achievement throughout childhood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 41(1), 94-104. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025415603489

*Lerner, R. M. (2018). Concepts and theories of human development. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203581629

*Li, M., Riis, J. L., Ghazarian, S. R., & Johnson, S. B. (2017). Income, family context, and self-regulation in 5-year-old children. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 38(2), 99-108. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0000000000000380

Lipina, S. (2021). Pobre cerebro. Siglo Veintiuno Editores.

*Lipina, S., & Segretin, M. S. (2015). 6000 días más: evidencia neurocientífica acerca del impacto de la pobreza infantil. Psicología Educativa, 21(2), 107-116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pse.2015.08.003

*Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B., Gunnar, M., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 434-445. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2639

*Masuda, T., Ellsworth, P., Mesquita, B., Leu, J., Tanida, S., & van de Veerdonk, E. (2008). Placing the face in context: Cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(3), 365-381. https://doi.org/b6tk6w

*McCoy, D., Peet, E., Ezzati, M., Danaei, G., Black, M., Sudfeld, C., Fawzi, W., & Fink, G. (2017). Early childhood developmental status in low-and middle-income countries: National, regional, and global prevalence estimates using predictive modelling. PLoS Medicine, 14(1), e1002233. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002233

*Morales-Nava, R., González-Arreola, M., & Chávez-Soto, B. (2021). Maestras de preescolar: características reconocidas en niños y niñas con aptitudes sobresalientes. Revista Latinoamericana de Ciencias Sociales, Niñez y Juventud, 19(3), 1-21. https://doi.org/j6q4

*Miguel, P., Pereira, L., Silveira, P., & Meaney, M. (2019). Early environmental influences on the development of children’s brain structure and function. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 61(10), 1127-1133. https://doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.14182

*Nisbett, R. E., & Miyamoto, Y. (2005). The influence of culture: Holistic versus analytic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 467-473. https://doi.org/dwqm9t

*Ortega, I., & Ruetti, E. (2014). La memoria del niño en la etapa prescolar. Anuario de Investigaciones, 21(2), 267-276.

*Page, M., McKenzie, J., Bossuyt, P., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T., Mulrow, C., Shamseer, L., Tetzlaff, J., Akl, E., Brennan, S., Chou, R., Glanville, J., Grimshaw, J., Hróbjartsson, A., Lalu, M., Li, T., Loder, E., Mayo-Wilson, E., McDonald, S., … Moher, D. (2021). The Prisma 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ, 372(71), 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.n71

*Patino, C. M., & Ferreira, J. C. (2018). Inclusion and exclusion criteria in research studies: definitions and why they matter. Jornal Brasileiro de Pneumologia, 44(2), 84-93. https://doi.org/10.1590/s1806-37562018000000088

*Peterson, C., Wang, Q., & Hou, Y. (2009). When I was little: Childhood recollections in Chinese and European Canadian grade school children. Child Development, 80(2), 506-518. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01275.x

*Rangaswami, S. (2021). Nature, nurture and the learning brain. En K. A. Thomas, J. V. Kureethara, & S. Bhattacharyya (Eds.) Neuro-systemic applications in learning (pp. 333- 368). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-72400-9_17

*Redshaw, J., Suddendorf, T., Neldner, K., Wilks, M., Tomaselli, K., Mushin, I., & Nielsen, M. (2019). Young children from three diverse cultures spontaneously and consistently prepare for alternative future possibilities. Child Development, 90(1), 51-61. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13084

*Reynolds, C., Altmann, R., & Allen, D. N. (2021). The problem of bias in psychological assessment. En Mastering modern psychological testing, (pp. 573-613). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-59455-8_15

*Roy, A. L., McCoy, D. C., & Raver, C. C. (2014). Instability versus quality: Residential mobility, neighborhood poverty, and children’s self-regulation. Developmental Psycho-logy, 50(7), 1891-1896. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036984

*Sabates, R., & Dex, S. (2015). The impact of multiple risk factors on young children’s cognitive and behavioural development. Children & Society, 29(2), 95-108. https:// doi.org/10.1111/chso.12024

*Sánchez-Reyes, J., Cantor-Jiménez, J., Castro-Sardi, X., & Bolaños, Y. (2019). Concepciones de niño y modos de relación de cuidadores con niños institucionalizados. Revista Latinoamericana de Ciencias Sociales, Niñez y Juventud, 17(2), 1-24. https://doi.org/j4fq

*Schober, P., & Vetter, T. R. (2019). Sample size and power in clinical research. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 129(2), 323-333. https://doi.org/10.1213/ANE.0000000000004316

*Seidler, A. L., & Ritchie, S. J. (2018). The association between socioeconomic status and cognitive development in children is partly mediated by a chaotic home atmosphere. Journal of Cognition and Development, 19(5), 486-508. https://doi.org/j6rn

*Sidhu, M., Malhi, P., & Jerath, J. (2010). Intelligence of children from economically disad-vantaged families: Role of parental education. Psychological Studies, 55(4), 358-364. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-010-0016-3

*Skolnick, A. (1975). The limits of childhood: Conceptions of child development and social context. Law and Contemporary Problems, 39(3), 38-77. https://doi.org/10.2307/1191268

*Spector, P. E. (2019). Do not cross me: Optimizing the use of cross-sectional designs. Journal of Business and Psychology, 34, 125-137. https://doi.org/ggsq7f

*Tam, K.-P., & Milfont, T. L. (2020). Towards cross-cultural environmental psychology: A state-of-the-art review and recommendations. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 71, 101474. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101474

*Tomopoulos, S., Dreyer, B., Berkule, S., Fierman, A., Brockmeyer, C., & Mendelsohn, A. (2010). Infant media exposure and toddler development. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 164(12), 1105-1111. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.235

*Tyler, F. B. (2020). Cross-cultural Psychology: Is it time to revise the model? En J. Walter, D. L. Lonner, D. K. Dinnel, S. A. Forgays (Eds.), Merging past, present, and future in cross-cultural Psychology (pp. 116-123). Garland Science. https://doi.org/j6rq

*Vázquez-Salas, A., Hubert, C., Villalobos, A., Sánchez-Ferrer, J., Ortega-Olvera, C., Romero, M., & Barrientos-Gutiérrez, T. (2020). Características infantiles y contextuales asociadas con el desarrollo infantil temprano en la niñez mexicana. Salud Pública de México, 62(6), 714-724. https://doi.org/10.21149/11869

*Veldhorst, C., Vervloed, M., Kef, S., & Steenbergen, B. (2022). A scoping review of longitudinal studies of children with vision impairment. British Journal of Visual Impairment, https://doi.org/10.1177/02646196211072432

*Vikram, K., & Chindarkar, N. (2020). Bridging the gaps in cognitive achievement in India: The crucial role of the integrated child development services in early childhood. World Development, 127, 104697. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.worlddev.2019.104697

*Wu, X., Cheng, G., Tang, C., Xie, Q., He, S., Li, R., & Yan, Y. (2020). The Effect of parenting quality on child development at 36-48 months in China’s urban area: Evidence from a birth cohort study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(23), 8962. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17238962

*Yoshida, H., & Smith, L. B. (2003). Shifting ontological boundaries: How Japanese-and English-speaking children generalize names for animals and artifacts. Developmental Science, 6(1), 1-17. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00247_1